perm filename OBJMEA[4,KMC]1 blob
sn#085743 filedate 1974-02-05 generic text, type T, neo UTF8
00100 THE OBJECTIVE MEASUREMENT OF AUTISTIC CHILDRENS' INTERACTION
00200 PATTERNS WITH A COMPUTER-CONTROLLED PROGRAM FOR
00300 THE STIMULATION OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
00400
00500
00600 KENNETH MARK COLBY
00700 AND
00800 HELENA C. KRAEMER
00900
01000 One difficulty in evaluating treatment improvements in
01100 disorders whose defining characteristics are purely behavioral is
01200 that we lack objective measurements. An objective measurement is one
01300 which is intersubjectively confirmable and impartially independent of
01400 individual opinions, intuitions, and judgemments. The fallibilities
01500 of humans trying to be both participants in and observer-recorders
01600 of, their interactions are such that objective measurement is
01700 impossible. But when one participant in an interaction is a machine
01800 such as a computer, an opportunity arises for the machine itself to
01900 record observations and to collect data. This capacity of a computer
02000 allows us to develop a measurement standard , a basis of comparison
02100 in which interaction patterns can be considered similar or different
02200 according to the measurement criteria.
02300 While developing a computer-aided treatment method for
02400 stimulating language behavior in nonspeaking autistic children (Colby
02500 and Smith,1971,Colby,1973), we became interested in the interaction
02600 patterns with the machine characteristic of these children and how
02700 they differed from normal children and others types of nonspeaking
02800 children. To evaluate the treatment method we attempted to follow
02900 interaction-pattern changes over time and to assess whether these
03000 changes could be considered as "improvement". If no changes occurred
03100 or if the changes were judged as "worsening", then the treatment
03200 should be discontinued. We chose to define improvement as a change in
03300 the interaction pattern towards the pattern characteristic of normal
03400 speaking children in particular age groups.
03500 The treatment involves letting a child play with a
03600 computer-controlled audio-visual device consisting of a
03700 typewriter-like keyboard and a television-like video display screen.
03800 When a child presses a key, a symbol, letter, word, expression or
03900 drawing appears on the screen accompanied by a sound, mainly human
04000 voice sounds and some animal or machine sounds. The overall program
04100 is divided into "games" of varying complexity. A sitter who sits with
04200 the child changes the games in accordance with the child's interests
04300 and abilities. The simplest game is Game #1 in which pressing a key
04400 produces that key's symbol on the video screen accompanied by a voice
04500 pronouncing the corresponding letter or number. We shall limit our
04600 discussion of objective measurement to the data collected in this one
04700 game.
04800 To keep track of the child's interactions with the machine, a
04900 program was written (by Earl Sacerdoti, a graduate student in the
05000 Department of Computer Science, Stanford University) which recorded
05100 the game the child was playing, which key was pressed at what exact
05200 time and whether the sound for this key was played over the audio
05300 device. The computer-controlled system is designed so that if a
05400 child presses a single key or several keys in bursts of less than a
05500 second's duration, the sound for the first pressing is played but the
05600 rest are not in order to avoid confusing the child. As soon as the
05700 child pauses at the end of such a burst, a buffer is cleared of all
05800 the symbols accumulated during the burst, and when the next key is
05900 pressed, its sound is played. Striking the keys at an extremely rapid
06000 rate indicates a child is ignoring the sound and paying attention
06100 only to his visual and/or tactile experience.
06200 In Fig. 1 the first column on the left indicates the number
06300 of the game, the second column indicates the key struck, the third
06400 column shows the time of striking in hours-minutes-seconds, and the
06500 fourth shows whether the sound for that key was played ("effective")
06600 or not (blank space). The data of Fig. 1 illustrates the interaction
06700 pattern of a nine year old nonspeaking boy clinically diagnosed as
06800 autistic and with a score of +26 on Rimland's E2 scale. (A score
06900 greater than +20 is considered by Rimland to indicate a true case of
07000 early infantile autism (Rimland,1971). Fig. 2 shows data from a
07100 normal 17 month old speaking boy. It is noteworthy how, on simple
07200 visual inspection, the pattern of the 9 year old autistic child
07300 resembles that of a 17 month old normal child. In this way an
07400 objective measurement allows us to establish equivalences between
07500 different children.
07600 To analyze data from several children playing Game #1, we
07700 examined 6 numerical scores for each child - the total number of runs
07800 (a run consisting of the same key being pressed), the length of the
07900 run, the average length of a run, the number of gaps (blank spaces
08000 indicating non-listening to sound), the length of the gap, and the
08100 average length of a gap. The data for these measures from normal and
08200 autistic children appear in Tables 1 and 2.
08300 From the normal children data we can construct a curve as
08400 shown in the graph of Fig. 3. Normal children from age 17 months to
08500 10 years progress in their interaction patterns along this curve. The
08600 interaction patterns of a nonspeaking autistic child locate him
08700 somewhere on this curve. Over time it can be estimated whether he is
08800 progressing towards a more normal pattern, whether he has reached a
08900 plateau or whether he is retrogressing. We have not yet observed
09000 the latter case. Since the data of the curve is longitudinal, it
09100 would be ideal if we could show the progress in normal and
09200 nonspeaking autistic groups of children over 10 year periods. Since
09300 we have only cross-sectional data thus far, this is what we used to
09400 construct the curve.
09500 As yet we do not have complete data from start to finish of
09600 treatment on an improved case of a nonspeaking autistic dhild.
09700 However, Fig. 4 shows the current (Aug.l973) interaction pattern of
09800 J, a 10 year old boy whom we treated for two years and whose language
09900 development gained markedly. (We were not collecting this type of
10000 data when he was in treatment.) Stretches of J's interaction patterns
10100 are quite normal looking and his location on the interaction-pattern
10200 curve can be seen in the graph of Fig.3. We have several cases
10300 of failure in which the final sessions of treatment show interaction
10400 patterns indistinguishable from those of the early sessions. The
10500 data of M. (see Fig. 1) reveal no change over the past two years. We
10600 will try for another year and if no change occurs, we will
10700 discontinue. We must also be prepared for the possibilities that (1)
10800 a child's comprehension and speech improves as judged by clinical and
10900 parental observation but his interaction patterns do not change or
11000 (2) a child's interaction patterns change towards the normal but he
11100 still does not use speech for social communication. As yet, we have
11200 not observed either of these paradoxical outcomes.
11400 In summary, we have presented an objective measurement of
11500 children's interaction patterns in playing with a computer-controlled
11600 audio-visual device programmed to stimulate language development.
11700 This measurement is useful in 3 ways: (1) it reveals where a child
11800 stands on an interaction curve relative to normal and other non-
11900 speaking autistic children; thus treatment can be planned suited to
12000 the child's position on the curve (2) changes in the interaction
12100 pattern over time can be evaluated to see if a child is improving or
12200 not, and (3) if no change takes place or a child reaches a plateau
12300 and remains there for a long time, discontinuation of the treatment
12400 can be justified. Thus an objective measurement of interaction
12500 patterns serves as a useful instrument in planning and terminating
12600 treatment of language deficiencies in nonspeaking autistic children.
12700 References
12800
12900 Colby, K. M. The rationale for computer-based treatment of
13000 language difficulties in nonspeaking autistic children.
13100 Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia,l973, 3, 254-260.
13300
13400 Colby, K. M. and Smith, D. C. Computers in the treatment
13500 of nonspeaking autistic children. In J. H. Masserman
13600 (Ed.), Current Psychiatric Therapies, Greene & Stratton,
13700 N.Y., 1971.
13800
13900 Rimland, B. The differentiation of childhood psychoses: an
14000 analysis of checklists for 2,218 children. Journal of
14100 Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1971, 1, 175-189.